How we play together

Psychologists from the Cluster of Excellence Centre for the Advanced Study of Collective Behaviour are using EEG to research what games reveal about our ability to cooperate.
© Universität Konstanz, Elisabeth Böker

Intense focus pervades the EEG laboratory at the University of Konstanz on this day of experimentation. In separate labs, two participants, connected by screens, engage in the computer game Pacman. The burning question: Can strangers, unable to communicate directly, synchronize their efforts to conquer the digital realm together?

© Universität Konstanz, CasCB

“Our research revolves around cooperative behaviour and the adoption of social roles.”

Karl-Philipp Flösch, doctoral candidate

Doctoral candidate Karl-Philipp Flösch is leading today's experiment. However, understanding brain processes underlying cooperative behaviour is still in its infancy, presenting a central challenge for cognitive neuroscience. How can cooperative behaviour be brought into a highly structured EEG laboratory environment without making it feel artificial or boring for study participants?

Pacman as a scientific “playground”

The research team, led by Harald Schupp, Professor of Biological Psychology at the University of Konstanz, envisioned using the well-known computer game Pacman as a natural medium to study cooperative behaviour in the EEG laboratory. Conducting the study as part of the Cluster of Excellence Centre for the Advanced Study of Collective Behaviour, they recently published their findings in Psychophysiology.

"Pacman is a cultural icon. Many have navigated the voracious Pacman through mazes in their youth, aiming to devour fruits and outsmart hostile ghosts", reminisces Karl-Philipp Flösch. Collaborating with colleagues, co-author Tobias Flaisch adapted the game. In the EEG version, two players instead of one must collaboratively guide Pacman to the goal. Flaisch explains: "Success hinges on cooperative behaviour, as players must seamlessly work together."

“From the perspective of classical psychological research, the game combines various skills inherent in natural social situations.”

Harald Schupp, Professor of Biological Psychology

However, the researchers have built in a special hurdle: the labyrinth's path is concealed. Only one of the two players can see where Pacman is going next. Flösch elaborates: “The active player can communicate the direction to the partner, but only indirectly using pre-agreed symbols, communicated solely through the computer screen." If you do not remember quickly enough that a crescent moon on the screen means that Pacman should move right, and that only the banana on the keyboard can make Pacman move to the right, you’re making a mistake.

© Flösch, Flaisch, Imhof, Schupp 2023

Schematic depiction of the Pacman Game. Game setup (a): Participants play a dyadic game while 128 channel EEG is measured simultaneously from both players. The players see each other via video stream and communicate by sending symbolic picture cues (e.g., bear + lemon = left). Experimental trial (b): For each game round, one player is informed about the upcoming moving direction. The informed player acts as sender of Picture 1, i.e., selecting a picture cue (e.g., the bear) to communicate the next moving direction (e.g., left) to the receiver, who awaits the presentation of the selected cue. After presentation of Picture 1, the receiver responds by selecting the complementary picture (e.g., the lemon), thus reversing the roles of the two players and becoming the sender of Picture 2.

EEG measures event-related potentials

During each game, the players' brain reactions were measured using EEG. Calculating event-related potentials provides a detailed view of the effects elicited by different game roles with millisecond-level temporal precision. The team hypothesized that the game role significantly influences brain reactions. Therefore, they examined the P3 component, a well-studied brain reaction exhibiting a stronger deflection in the presence of significant and task-relevant stimuli. The results confirmed their assumption: "The P3 was increased not only when the symbol indicated the next move's direction but also when observing whether the game partner selected the correct symbol", says Flösch. The team concludes that the role we take on during cooperation determines the informational value of environmental stimuli situationally. EEG measurements allow the brain processes involved to be dynamically mapped.

https://youtu.be/swvfsgnEkp8

Cooperative role adoption structures society

"Cooperative role adoption structures our entire society", summarizes Schupp, providing context for the study. "An individual achieves little alone, but collectively, humanity even reaches the moon. Our technological society hinges on cooperative behavior", says Flösch, adding that children early on take individual roles, thereby learning the art of complex cooperation. Consequently, this role adoption occurs nearly effortlessly and automatically for us every day. "Our brains are practically 'built' for it, as evidenced by the results of our study."  

 

Elisabeth Böker

By Elisabeth Böker - 20.11.2023